Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine
  Home | About Us | Editorial Board | Search | Ahead of print | Current Issue | Archives | Instructions | Contact | Advertise | Submission | Login 
Users Online: 169 
Wide layoutNarrow layoutFull screen layoutHome Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font sizeIncrease font size


 
 Table of Contents    
CASE REPORT
Year : 2012  |  Volume : 34  |  Issue : 4  |  Page : 385-387  

Intelligence and Cognition in a Child with High Functioning Autism


Centre for Health Psychology, University of Hyderabad, Hyderabad, Andhra Pradesh, India

Date of Web Publication7-Mar-2013

Correspondence Address:
Thomas M Kishore
Centre for Health Psychology, University of Hyderabad, Hyderabad 500 046, Andhra Pradesh
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/0253-7176.108228

Rights and Permissions
   Abstract 

Intelligence is assessed for ruling out mental retardation and to find out the relative cognitive strengths in autism. Of special interest is to know the nature of intelligence and cognition in high functioning autism. But very little is known how the assessments are carried given the deficits in communication, socialization in autism. This cross-sectional study aims to describe the nature of intelligence and cognition in a child with HFA and drawing implications for assessment in the Indian setting. Results indicate that there is no evidence for superior crystallized intelligence in HFA, though a jagged profile could be expected both across and within cognitive domains.

Keywords: Assessment process, cognition, high functioning autism, intelligence


How to cite this article:
Kishore TM. Intelligence and Cognition in a Child with High Functioning Autism. Indian J Psychol Med 2012;34:385-7

How to cite this URL:
Kishore TM. Intelligence and Cognition in a Child with High Functioning Autism. Indian J Psychol Med [serial online] 2012 [cited 2019 Oct 18];34:385-7. Available from: http://www.ijpm.info/text.asp?2012/34/4/385/108228


   Introduction Top


A significant proportion of individuals with autism will have either comorbid mental retardation [1] or atypical cognitive processes. [2] Hence intelligence assessment is necessary to rule out both. But as in case of people with high functioning autism (HFA), defined operationally as those with an intelligence quotient (IQ) of 70 and above, [3] the same may help us understand the cognitive resources in light of autistic features. For this reason there is a considerable interest in the nature of both cognition and intelligence in HFA. There is a general view that people with HFA will have an intact or superior functioning in selective attention, rule-learning, visuospatial areas, motor learning, and perceptual motor integration. [1],[4],[5],[6] With regards to intelligence, verbal quotient was usually reported to be higher than the performance quotient [7] lending support to a conventional hypothesis that probably superior crystallized intelligence is a characteristic feature of HFA. [8] Nevertheless there are no studies from India to corroborate the above findings. In this context, the present case study aimed to describe the nature of intelligence and cognition in a child with HFA and drawing implications for assessment in the Indian setting.


   Case Report Top


SA, a 5-year and 7-month-old boy was brought by his parents with the complaints of delayed speech. Birth history was uneventful though there was significant delay in attaining meaningful speech, communicable phrases, and social skills. Medical and therapeutic assessments ruled out sensory-motor impairments and significant organic brain pathology. Behavioral observation revealed that he was neat and tidy; continuously curling up in chair, flapping, and vocalizing. He needed repeated prompts for both arousing and sustaining attention. He followed repetitive commands very mechanically. His eye contact was brief, deviant and at times, vacant. His affect ranged from normal state to an inappropriately cheerful state. Rapport could not be developed. He demonstrated little pragmatic social skills or any age-appropriate gestures. Based on this presentation, an International Classification of Diseases, Version 10 diagnosis of autism was made. Further rating on Childhood Autism Rating Scale [9] by psychiatrist indicated that the child had severe autism. Asperger's disorder was ruled out by applying Gilliam Asperger's Disorder Scale. [10] Special educator's report indicated that he could simplify ratios and identify degree of given area or angles in circle and triangle, respectively in written form. He could even solve some randomly selected arithmetic of grade VI and VII. But he could not apply these skills in a social context or for functional purposes. Similar performance was not reported or identified in other subjects.

Assessment procedure

Developmental Screening Test (DST) [11] and Vineland Social Maturity Scale (VSMS) [12] were administered first to reduce the test anxiety and also to gain insight about applicability of intelligence scales based on the developmental profile. On DST his developmental age was 2 year and 10 months and the developmental quotient was 52; and on VSMS his social age was 3 years and the social quotient of 55 [Figure 1].
Figure 1: Vineland social maturity scale profile. SHG - Self-help general; SHE - Self-help eating; SHD - Self-help dressing; SD - Self-direction; OCC - Occupation; COM - Communication; LOC - Locomotion; SOC - Socialization

Click here to view


Intelligence assessment was carried in a quiet room in a single day, with adequate breaks suiting the child's attention, motivation. Gessell's Drawing Test [13] was used as a screening measure of intelligence. Since he did not copy beyond the first design (i.e., circle) the test was suspended and his mental age was estimated to be around 3 years. Binet-Kamat Test of Intelligence (BKT) [14] yielded a basal age of 6 years, ceiling age of 19 years, and a mental age of 10 year and 6 months, though he passed items up to 16 years. The corresponding IQ was 188. He did not have any problem in attention to specific tasks nor in immediate recall and short-term memory. He failed on several visuospatial tasks whereas a better performance was noted for non-meaningful memory, conceptual thinking, social intelligence, and numerical reasoning. Further, the profile analysis of BKT based on the description of Lezak [15] indicated that the performance across various cognitive domains was very uneven albeit with relatively equal performance on verbal items (60%) and non-verbal items (57%) [Figure 2].
Figure 2: Binet– Kamat test of intelligence profile. The graph is not continuous as the child did not pass any item in non-verbal reasoning domain, where the items start at 12 years

Click here to view


Nevertheless, certain adaptations were required for administering BKT. For example, items requiring verbal responses were tested through written form to understand the level and quality of his conceptual thinking. For example, he could not say a sentence with three key words, a skill expected by 9 years age, but expressed the same in writing. The only qualitative difference was that the letters were as large as two inch and were poorly aligned. However, responses were noted but not credited when they had to be in specific form. For example, no credit was given when the child failed to repeat sentences as part of testing.


   Discussion Top


Studying intelligence and cognition in persons with HFA may offer insights about cognitive resources in the backdrop of autism. Contrary to previous studies, [8] HFA may not always be characterized by superior crystallized intelligence. Rather there could be an uneven performance across subtests of IQ measures, which is already acknowledged. [1] Additionally, this study indicates that there could be intra-domain unevenness, as noted particularly in the areas of attention, short-term memory, and visuospatial skills. That is, a superior skill can be noted in these areas despite a failure on considerably lower tasks. However, further validation is required to understand whether it is a limitation of the test or a characteristic feature of HFA, though the response pattern indicates a possibility of the latter.

A superior performance on tasks tapping selective attention together with complexity-dependent memory deficits but not modality-dependent memory deficits; and modality specific language deficits and poor social cognition only adds to the view that cognitive deficits in autism are not primarily due to attention deficits. [16] Despite a very high level of intelligence, failure on items related to repeating complex sentences, recalling stories, and adaptive behavior was also found in previous studies. [17],[18]

This study also has specific implications for assessment, that is, global intelligence scales could be applied even when the screening measures and behavioral observation indicate inferior skill repertoire. Further, lack of appropriate communication skills does not rule out the ability to express concepts in writing. Therefore, clinicians may uncover the conceptual levels in HFA by allowing them to express by writing. This kind of adaptation is necessary keeping in view that age and IQ-appropriate writing skills may distinguish autism from other developmental disorders. [17]

In conclusion, intact or superior crystallized intelligence is not necessarily found in all cases of HFA. Uneven performance may be found not only across cognitive domains but also within a cognitive domain. Global intelligence scales could be applied to understand this pattern even when behavioral observations and screening measures indicate a poor response pattern. Future studies may compare the profiles of intelligence, cognition, and language in children with HFA with that of the typically developing children, and children with specific speech and language impairments to understand cognitive markers of autism.

 
   References Top

1.Eigsti IM, Shapiro T. A systems neuroscience approach to autism: Biological, cognitive, and clinical perspectives. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2003;9:205-15.  Back to cited text no. 1
[PUBMED]    
2.Dawson M, Soulières I, Gernsbacher MA, Mottron L. The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychol Sci 2007;18:657-62.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.Goldstein G, Allen DN, Minshew NJ, Williams DL, Volkmar F, Klin A, et al. The structure of intelligence in children and adults with high functioning autism. Neuropsychology 2008;22:301-12.  Back to cited text no. 3
[PUBMED]    
4.Bölte S, Poustka F. Comparing the intelligence profiles of savant and nonsavant individuals with autistic disorder. Intelligence 2004;32:121-31.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.Minshew NJ, Goldstein G. Autism as a disorder of complex information processing. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 1998;4:129-36.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.Smalley SL, Asarnow RF. Cognitive subclinical markers in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1990;20:271-8.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.Boucher J. Spectrum Model of Autism: Concept and Definitions. In: The Autistic Spectrum: characteristics, Causes and Practical Issues. Los Angeles: Sage; 2009. p. 22-41.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.Lincoln AJ, Courchesne E, Kilman BA, Elmasian R, Allen M. A study of intellectual abilities in high-functioning people with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1988;18:505-24.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.Schopler E, Reichler RJ, Renner BR. The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2002.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.Gilliam JE. Gilliam Asperger Disorder Scale. Austin, TX: PRO-ED; 2001.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.Bharatraj J. Developmental Screening Test. Mysore: Swayam Sidha Prakashanam; 1977.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.Bharatraj J. Vineland Social Maturity Scale-Indian Adaptation: Enlarged Version. Mysore: Swayamsiddha Prakashanam; 1992.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.Verma SK, Pershad D, Kaushal P. Gessell Drawing Test as a measure of intelligence in the mentally retarded children. Indian J Med Res 1972;5:64-8.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.Kamat VV. Measuring Intelligence of Indian Children. 4 th ed. London: Oxford University Press; 1967.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1983.   Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.Green L, Fein D, Joy S, Waterhouse L. Cognitive functioning in autism: An overview. In: Schopler E, Mesibov GB, editors. Learning and Cognition in Autism. New York: Plenum Press; 1995. p. 13-31.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.Boucher J. Memory for recent events in autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord 1981;11:293-301.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ. The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology 2006;20:21-9.  Back to cited text no. 18
    


    Figures

  [Figure 1], [Figure 2]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
  
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
    Abstract
   Introduction
   Case Report
   Discussion
    References
    Article Figures

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed3141    
    Printed60    
    Emailed1    
    PDF Downloaded100    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal