Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine
  Home | About Us | Editorial Board | Search | Ahead of print | Current Issue | Archives | Instructions | Contact | Advertise | Submission | Login 
Users Online: 1289 
Wide layoutNarrow layoutFull screen layoutHome Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font sizeIncrease font size


 
 Table of Contents    
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2019  |  Volume : 41  |  Issue : 3  |  Page : 246-251  

Neuro-Cognition in adolescents with dissociative disorder: A study from a Tertiary Care Center of North India


Department of Psychiatry, King George's Medical University, Lucknow, Uttar Pradesh, India

Date of Submission17-Mar-2018
Date of Acceptance03-Jan-2019
Date of Web Publication17-May-2019

Correspondence Address:
Dr. Sujita K Kar
Department of Psychiatry, King George's Medical University, Lucknow - 226 003, Uttar Pradesh
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/IJPSYM.IJPSYM_127_18

Rights and Permissions
   Abstract 


Background: Dissociative disorder is a common neurotic disorder. Patients with dissociative disorder experience significant psychological distress and have deficits in various domains of neurocognitive functions. Objective: To assess the neurocognitive functioning of adolescents diagnosed with dissociative disorder and compare it with that of healthy controls. Materials and Methods: This is a cross-sectional observational study conducted on adolescents diagnosed with dissociative disorder, attending child and adolescent specialty clinic of a tertiary care hospital of North India from October 2016 to February 2017. Healthy control subjects were also recruited for comparison on study variables. Malin's Intelligence Scale for Indian children and standardized neuropsychological tools were administered for the assessment of intellectual functioning and neurocognitive functioning. Results: A total of 50 participants with dissociative disorder and 50 healthy controls completed the study. Participants of both the groups had an average level of intellectual functioning. Participants with dissociative disorder showed poorer performance on tasks of attention and executive functions. After the Bonferroni correction, deficits were detected in the domains of coding (P = 0.0012), maze (P = 0.0001), and mathematics (P = 0.0016). Conclusions: Adolescents with dissociative disorder have impaired neurocognitive functions in comparison to healthy controls.

Keywords: Adolescents, dissociative disorder, neuro-cognition
Key messages: a) Adolescents with dissociative disorder have deficits of certain neurocognitive functions in comparison to healthy control. b) Though the overall intellectual functioning of patients with dissociative disorder are comparable with that of healthy controls, significant deficits remain in the domains of coding, arithmetic, and maze task.


How to cite this article:
Dixit A, Singh S, Kar SK, Arya A, Agarwal V. Neuro-Cognition in adolescents with dissociative disorder: A study from a Tertiary Care Center of North India. Indian J Psychol Med 2019;41:246-51

How to cite this URL:
Dixit A, Singh S, Kar SK, Arya A, Agarwal V. Neuro-Cognition in adolescents with dissociative disorder: A study from a Tertiary Care Center of North India. Indian J Psychol Med [serial online] 2019 [cited 2019 Sep 16];41:246-51. Available from: http://www.ijpm.info/text.asp?2019/41/3/246/257382



The recently conducted National Mental Health Survey (2015–16) had revealed, in a community representative population, the prevalence of mental morbidity in adolescents aged between 13 and 17 years to be 7.3% in India.[1] The proportion of patients diagnosed with dissociative disorders in India ranged between 1.5–15.0 per 1000 outpatients and between 1.5–11.6 per 1000 inpatients. The prevalence rate of dissociative disorder in child and adolescent group in India has been reported to be 12.5%.[2]

Several studies have focused on the psychosocial variables related to patients suffering from dissociative disorder.[3],[4],[5],[6] The cognitive aspect of dissociative disorder is less understood. Recent studies have focused on the functional connectivity between the brain regions involved in emotion processing and those involved in representing sensory information, which may be enhanced, generating unusual motor sensory symptoms in the context of stress.[4],[7],[8]

A century ago, Janet (1919) was the first to conceptualize dissociative symptoms as having a neurocognitive component – in particular, as disorders of memory processing that arise in the wake of trauma. Later, neurobiological models pointed to deficits in both memory and attention and postulated that these deficits would be more prominent during the presence of symptoms and during testing conditions that were stressful or that provoked anxiety.[9],[10]

The importance of intelligence in the genesis of neurotic illness has been reported in the literature.[11],[12],[13] The intellectual functioning of patients with dissociative disorders has been studied, but the results are very inconsistent, as some studies report average intellectual functioning in dissociative disorders, whereas some other studies report decreased baseline intellectual quotient or presence of scatters on intellectual testing.[14],[15],[16] The presence of scatter on intelligence subtests indicates the presence of neurocognitive deficits which may be an important causal factor of dissociation. Studies on neurocognition in dissociative disorder had also showed mixed results. An Indian study conducted by Ranjan et al., (2016) on a sample of child and adolescent patients (8–16 years) suffering from dissociative disorder revealed the presence of significant deficits in the areas of visuoconstructive and visual organization abilities, verbal working memory, executive functioning, learning, and attention.[16] In another study in children and adolescents with conversion disorder, a similar pattern of neurocognitive deficits was found.[15]

Evidence suggests that children and adolescents with various subtypes of dissociative disorders exhibit impairments in spatial working memory, planning and organization, attention, verbal memory and naming tests, as well as decreased performance on tasks of visual memory and immediate memory.[17],[18],[19]

Most of the existing literature on patients with dissociative disorders are on the adult population. The evidence regarding the neurocognitive functioning in children and adolescents are inconclusive and inconsistent. We hypothesize that adolescents with dissociative disorder have deficits in neurocognitive functions (learning, memory, intelligence, visuospatial abilities, attention and concentration, and executive function) compared to healthy counterparts. The recent study of Ranjan et al., (2016) assessed neuropsychological functioning of children and adolescents with dissociative disorder.[16] But the study did not include a comparison with healthy controls. We adopted the concept of the above-mentioned study and aimed to assess the neurocognitive function of adolescent patients presenting with dissociative disorders and compared it with healthy controls.


   Materials and Methods Top


It is a cross-sectional observational study conducted in a tertiary care center of North India. The sample consisted of 50 adolescents aged between 10–16 years, with a diagnosis of dissociative disorder, and 50 age, gender, and education-matched healthy controls. It is a time-bound study of 5 months. The patients with dissociative disorder were recruited from the child and adolescent psychiatry clinic of a tertiary care hospital of North India. The diagnosis of dissociative disorder, consistent with ICD-10 criteria, was made by the consultant psychiatrist. Participants who were able to read and write as well as not having visual, speech, or hearing impairments were included in the study. Those with medical illnesses involving the central nervous system and those with mental retardation, indicated by clinical history and developmental milestones, were excluded.

Healthy controls (age, gender, and education-matched) were recruited from healthy siblings/relatives of non-psychotic, non-attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and non-autistic patients attending the psychiatry child and adolescent outpatient services in the same institute. They were healthy siblings of patients with either dissociative/conversion disorder, depression, or adjustment disorder. We adopted the selection criteria and domains of neuropsychological assessment from the study of Ranjan et al., (2016).[16]

Tools used for assessment

  1. Malin's Intelligence Scale for Indian Children (MISIC)[20]: The battery comprises of 11 tests viz, information, comprehension, similarities, digit span, arithmetic, vocabulary, picture completion, object assembly, mazes, coding, and block design. Assessment on MISIC, three different types of scores were generated i.e., verbal quotient (VQ), performance quotient (PQ), and full-scale IQ. VQ included information, comprehension, vocabulary, as well as the understanding of abstract. PQ included working memory, attention, planning and problem solving as well as visuospatial processing especially in the areas of attention, working memory, and planning and problem solving
  2. NIMHANS neuropsychological battery[21] (selective subtests): The battery is a description of neuropsychological tests in current usage internationally as well as their mode of administration and normative data for Indian subjects' neuropsychological functioning in a comprehensive manner. For the purpose of the present study, selective subtests were opted. The tests selected from the battery were – Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (set-shifting ability), Stroop Test (response inhibition), and Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (learning and memory).


Procedure

The study was conducted from October 2016 to February 2017, after getting ethical clearance from the institutional ethics committee. Patients and controls were recruited as per the selection criteria. Written informed consent and assent were sought from the legal guardians of the participants and all the participants included in the study, respectively. Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID) 6.0 version was applied to rule out the presence of any comorbid psychiatric disorder. Sociodemographic and clinical details and neurocognitive functioning of the subjects of both the groups were assessed using the above tools. At the time of assessment, participants with dissociative disorder were stable. Neuropsychological assessments were completed soon after the clinical assessment. To minimize the drug effect on neurocognitive functioning, efforts were taken to administer the test at least 6 h after the last dose of medication.

Data analysis

Data were obtained using the Microsoft Excel 2007 software. Statistical analysis was performed on the SPSS version-16.0. Student's independent t-tests were used to compare scores of intellectual functioning and neurocognition between the experimental and the control groups. As multiple tests were applied, Bonferroni correction was used, and P value at 0.004 (0.05/14) was considered significant.


   Results Top


The sample was drawn from 86 consecutive referrals of children presenting with dissociative disorder. Out of 86, only 50 (33 girls and 17 boys) fulfilled the selection criteria and were included in the study. Most of the subjects were females (66%). The mean ± SD age of patients with dissociative disorder (13.3 ± 1.54 years) and control group (13.57 ± 1.31 years) were comparable. Most of the participants were belonging to the urban background (60% of the patients with dissociative disorder and 58% of control group) and were studying in 9th–10th standard. Most of the subjects were Muslims (62% in the patients with dissociative disorder and 64% in the control group), living in joint families, and with monthly family income between Rs. 10,000–20,000. The patients with dissociative disorder and the control group belonged to a comparable sociodemographic background [Table 1].
Table 1: Sociodemographic profile of the participants

Click here to view


The mean duration of the current dissociative episode was 15.48 ± 8.63 days. Among the study population, 66% had mixed dissociative disorder, 18% had dissociative convulsions, 10% had dissociative stupor, and 6% had trance and possession disorder.

Assessment on Malin's Intelligence Scale for Indian Children

Patients with dissociative disorder and control group had comparable intellectual functioning in all the domains except performance quotient (t = 2.48, P = 0.0149). However, it was insignificant after applying the Bonferroni correction [Table 2].
Table 2: Comparison of experimental and control groups on verbal, performance, and full 78-scale intelligence quotient

Click here to view


Neurocognitive battery

Subjects of the patients with dissociative disorder showed difficulty related to attention as evident on the subtest of coding (t = 3.31, P = 0.001). The patients with dissociative disorder (92.22 ± 6.38) had significantly lower scores (t = 3.406, P = 0.001) on the subtest of attention (coding) as compared to the control group (95.59 ± 2.87). The patients with dissociative disorder as compared to the control group performed poorly on both the tests of working memory i.e., Digit Span Test (t = 2.19, P = 0.030) and mathematics (t = 3.24, P = 0.002). Compared to the control group, the patients with dissociative disorder was found to have poorer performance in subtest of similarities (t = 2.13, P = 0.035) as well as on the WCST (total correct response, P = 0.018; non-perseveratory errors, P = 0.012). Subjects of the patients with dissociative disorder showed decreased planning and problem-solving capacity (maze) (t = 4, P = 0.0001) as compared to the control group [Table 3].
Table 3: Comparison of experimental and control groups on MISIC subtests, WCST parameters, RAVLT, and stroop test

Click here to view


After applying the Bonferroni correction, a significant difference persisted only in the domains of coding (P = 0.001), maze (0.0001), and mathematics (P = 0.002). Other domains that were found significant after applying the t-test were nullified after application of Bonferroni correction.


   Discussion Top


In our study, the patients with dissociative disorder and the control group belonged to a comparable sociodemographic background; hence the influence of sociodemographic variables on the study outcome is unlikely.

Both the groups had an average level of intelligence. There are differences in verbal as well as performance quotient among the study population (study and control group), which were non-significant after Bonferroni correction. The presence of scatter in attainment on subtests of intelligence battery indicates significant neurocognitive deficits in certain areas in these patients. Previous studies also found that patients with dissociative disorder had an average level of intellectual functioning and significantly lower performance intelligence as compared to verbal intelligence.[15],[16]

In the present study, among neurocognitive tests, compared to the healthy controls, the subjects with dissociative disorder had impairment in the areas of attention, understanding of an abstract concept, and some aspects of executive functioning i.e., working memory, planning and problem solving, and set shifting ability. However, other neurocognitive areas like response inhibition ability, learning, memory, comprehension, visuospatial skills, and vocabulary were not different significantly among patients with dissociative disorder and healthy controls.

Each of the neurocognitive tasks on which dissociative disorder subjects performed poorly is dependent on adequate prefrontal cortical functioning.[22] It suggests that the pattern of cognitive difficulties shown by participants with dissociative symptoms is consistent with a disorder of cognitive control. The present study specifically focused on the adolescent population, and no significant deficits were found in visuoconstructive and visual organization abilities in the present study.

The construct of cognitive control is understood as comprising a number of partially independent functions, including proper allocation of attentional resources (measured here by coding Attention Test), conflict monitoring (WCST), response inhibition (Stroop test), working memory (Digit Span Test, Coding), and planning and problem solving ability (maze test). The decreased working memory capacity on multiple subtests of the Digit Span Test and Coding test showed a clear deficit in the cognitive component of prefrontal cortex (PFC) function. The increased number of errors on WCST highlights the participants' difficulties in blocking interfering information throughout these domains. After Bonferroni correction, significant differences remained on the domains of mathematics, maze, and coding; however, no significant difference was found in any of the parameters of WCST, indicating the need for further research in a larger population to assess the differences in cognitive parameters. With certainty, it can be said that patients with dissociative disorder have deficits in certain parameters of neuropsychological functioning, which may be taken as soft signs or cognitive markers. Tax the resources.

The function of the PFC is to utilize the limited resources to maintain an effective balance between cognition and emotional processing.[23] The interaction between cognitive function (executive function) and emotion regulation in the PFC reflects the intactness of functional connectivity of PFC with amygdala. The above interaction is a complex process, where the emotional stimuli are processed which in turn influence the cognitive performance. This interaction process in the PFC is mediated through allocation of resources. In situ ations where there is an increased demand (pressure to perform), resources may get depleted and imbalance may happen in cognitive and emotional control (due to poor allocation of resources to PFC for cognitive control).[24] Hence, cognitive control is implemented by the PFC and it functions to override emotional or habitual responses to stimuli.[24] Our cohort of adolescents with dissociative disorder showed decreased performance in the above-mentioned areas. As all of the above tasks involve the PFC, a shift in the balance between cognitive/integrative processing and emotion/motor-sensory processing secondary to long-term stress will compromise individuals' cognitive/integrative capacities. These findings are consistent with earlier studies which found the presence of deficits in attention, working memory, set shifting ability, as well as planning and problem solving in subjects with dissociative disorder as compared to healthy controls.[15],[16]

In this study, out of the 50 adolescents with dissociative disorders, none had a comorbid brain disease, and testing was completed soon after the presentation when the patients were receiving a low dosage of medication. In addition, patients with developmental delay were excluded, and the participants with dissociative disorders and control group were comparable in terms of their IQ. This was crucial, as multiple developmental pathways and several factors related to brain disorders that can potentially affect neurocognitive function in patients with dissociative disorder, like brain disorders (for instance, developmental delay, epilepsy, and other neurological abnormalities), childhood maltreatment, psychotropic medications, and medications for the treatment of comorbid epilepsy, had a relatively minor presence in this study. Thus, it is unlikely that they would have affected the performance of the patients with dissociative disorder as compared to the control group on these neuropsychological tests.

The present study highlights that mechanisms underpinning dissociative symptoms can be activated in what is, neurologically, a healthy brain. Thus, although brain disease may potentially be a risk factor for dissociative disorders, it cannot be considered as either a necessary or sufficient condition for their development. Adolescents with dissociative disorder have compromised PFC functions that mediate cognitive/integrative processing. This finding has important implications for therapy. The clinicians may assess the neurocognitive functioning of adolescents with dissociative disorder as it is the formative age during which the development of neurocognitive functions occurs.

Limitations

Small sample size and narrow age range selection limit the generalizability of the study findings. Though the patients were receiving very low doses of psychotropic medications (benzodiazepines or antidepressants), their effect on neurocognitive function could not be ruled out. Future studies could address these shortcomings by replicating the study on a bigger sample with a broader age range, to make the findings more generalizable. All subtypes of dissociative disorder were clubbed together due to the small sample in each subgroup. The diversity of neurocognitive dysfunction among these subgroups cannot be ruled out. The dissociative psychopathology among the patients was not quantified and hence not correlated with the neurocognitive deficits. Additionally, it would be worthwhile looking at the neuro-cognitive symptoms of patients with dissociative disorder in the remission phase. Healthy controls were healthy siblings/relatives of patients with depression, dissociative disorder, or adjustment disorder, which may limit the generalizability of the study. Choosing healthy controls from natural settings with no family members affected with psychiatric disorder might increase the generalizability of the findings. No structured screening tool was used, which is another limitation of the study.


   Conclusion Top


Adolescents with dissociative disorder have obvious neurocognitive deficits compared to their healthy counterparts. Understanding the pattern of neuropsychological deficits will help in understanding the disorder from the perspective of neuropsychology. It may guide the clinician for understanding the etiopathogenesis of the disorder as well as planning the therapeutic intervention.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
   References Top

1.
Gururaj G, Varghese M, Benegal V, Rao GN, Pathak K, Singh LK, et al. National Mental Health Survey of India, 2015-16: Prevalence, patterns and outcomes. Bengaluru, National Institute of Mental Health and Neuro Sciences, NIMHANS Publication No. 129, 2016.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Srinath S, Bharat S, Girimaji S, Seshadri S. Characteristics of a child inpatient population with hysteria in India. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993;32:822-5.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Brunner R, Parzer P, Schuld V, Resch F. Dissociative symptomatology and traumatogenic factors in adolescent psychiatric patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 2000;188:71-7.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Nilsson D, Svedin CG. Evaluation of the swedish version of dissociation questionnaire (DIS-Q), Dis-Q-Sweden, among adolescents. J Trauma Dissociation 2006;7:65-89.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Silberg JL, Dallam S. Dissociation in children and adolescents: At the crossroads. In: Dell PF, O'Neil JA, editors. Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond. New York: Routledge; 2009. p. 67.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Liotti G. Attachment and dissociation. In: Dell PF, O'Neil JA, editors. Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond. New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group; 2009. p. 53-65.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
van der Kruijs SJ, Bodde NM, Vaessen MJ, Lazeron RH, Vonck K, Boon P, et al. Functional connectivity of dissociation in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry 2012;83:239-47.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Cojan Y, Waber L, Schwartz S, Rossier L, Forster A, Vuilleumier P. The brain under self-control: Modulation of inhibitory and monitoring cortical networks during hypnotic paralysis. Neuron 2009;62:862-75.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Ludwig AM. Hysteria: A neurobiological theory. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972;27:771-7.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Whitlock FA. The aetiology of hysteria. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1967;43:144-62.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Nunn C, Bergmann K, Britton P, Foster E, Hall E, Kay D. Intelligence and neurosis in old age. Br J Psychiatry 1974;124:446-52.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Britton P, Bergmann K, Kay D, Savage R. Mental state, cognitive functioning, physical health, and social class in the community aged. J Gerontol 1967;22:517-21.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Slater ET, Glithero E. A follow-up of patients diagnosed as suffering from “hysteria”. J Psychosom Res 1965;9:9-13.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Sackellares J, Iasemidis L, Gilmore R, Roper S. Epileptic seizures as neural resting mechanisms. Epilepsia 1997;38:189.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Kozlowska K, Palmer DM, Brown KJ, Scher S, Chudleigh C, Davies F, et al. Conversion disorder in children and adolescents: A disorder of cognitive control. J Neuropsychol 2015;9:87-108.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Ranjan R, Mehta M, Sagar R, Sarkar S. Relationship of cognitive function and adjustment difficulties among children and adolescents with dissociative disorder. J Neurosci Rural Pract 2016;7:238-43.  Back to cited text no. 16
  [Full text]  
17.
Almis BH, Cumurcu BE, Unal S, Ozcan AC, Aytas O. The neuropsychological and neurophysiological profile of women with pseudoseizure. Compr Psychiatry 2013;54:649-57.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Strutt AM, Hill SW, Scott BM, Uber-Zak L, Fogel TG. A comprehensive neuropsychological profile of women with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav 2011;20:24-8.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Binder LM, Campbell KA. Medically unexplained symptoms and neuropsychological assessment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004;26:369-92.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
AJ. M. Malin's intelligence scale for Indian children. Indian J Men Retard 1971;4:15-25.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Kar BR, Rao S, Chandramouli B, Thennarasu K. NIMHANS neuropsychological battery for children-manual. Bangalore: NIMHANS publication division; 2004.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Kane MJ, Engle RW. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev 2002;9:637-71.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Pessoa L. How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn Sci 2009;13:160-6.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Lindström BR, Bohlin G. Threat-relevance impairs executive functions: Negative impact on working memory and response inhibition. Emotion 2012;12:384.  Back to cited text no. 24
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
  
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
    Abstract
   Materials and Me...
     Results
     Discussion
     Conclusion
    References
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed324    
    Printed12    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded54    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal