Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine
  Home | About Us | Editorial Board | Search | Ahead of print | Current Issue | Archives | Instructions | Contact | Advertise | Submission | Login 
Users Online: 399 
Wide layoutNarrow layoutFull screen layoutHome Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font sizeIncrease font size


 
 Table of Contents    
BRIEF RESEARCH COMMUNICATION
Year : 2019  |  Volume : 41  |  Issue : 4  |  Page : 375-379  

Self-reported anger: Vulnerability for risky behaviors in two-wheeler riding young men


1 Department of Clinical Psychology, National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bengaluru, Karnataka, India
2 Traffic Engineering and Safety Division, CSIR-Central Road Research Institute, New Delhi, India

Date of Submission08-Oct-2018
Date of Acceptance08-Jan-2019
Date of Web Publication15-Jul-2019

Correspondence Address:
Paulomi M Sudhir
Department of Clinical Psychology, National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bengaluru, Karnataka
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/IJPSYM.IJPSYM_414_18

Rights and Permissions
   Abstract 


Objectives: Aggressive driving and road accidents are major concerns in the public health sector. This study aimed to explore risk to aggressive and risky behaviors on the road in two-wheeler riding young men. Methods: The study comprised 433 young male two-wheeler riders from an urban city of India. A two-wheeler riding survey that captured subjective perception of difficulty in managing anger in general, easy provocability to anger, and aggressive and risky behaviors on the road, and Negative Mood Regulation (NMR) scale were administered. Results: Of the 433 participants, 83 (19%) reported experiencing problematic anger in general, whereas 175 (40.42%) did not endorse experiencing problematic anger. Based on this, two groups were formed, namely, problematic anger-present group and problematic anger-absent group. The problematic anger-present group reported high score on easy provocability to anger, difficulty in controlling anger, specific motives related to riding fast than usual, and severity of aggressive responses to frustrating situations while riding, and low score on NMR scale. Statistical analysis revealed a significant difference between the groups. Conclusion: This study highlights the relevance of assessing subjective perception of problematic anger in two-wheeler riding young men. This has implications for designing interventions for enhancing road safety.

Keywords: Aggressive driving, anger, negative mood regulation, road rage, two-wheeler riding
Key messages: (a) Assessment of subjective perception of difficulty in managing anger in general could be very useful for understanding the aggressive and risky driving behaviours. (b) The study highlights the potential utility of identification of problematic anger and addressing the same. (c) Difficulty in managing anger in general and negative mood regulation. (d) Findings have implications for designing interventions aimed at minimizing risky driving behaviours and enhancing road safety.


How to cite this article:
Kumar R, Sudhir PM, Michael RJ, Sharma MK, Chakrabarty N, Mehrotra S. Self-reported anger: Vulnerability for risky behaviors in two-wheeler riding young men. Indian J Psychol Med 2019;41:375-9

How to cite this URL:
Kumar R, Sudhir PM, Michael RJ, Sharma MK, Chakrabarty N, Mehrotra S. Self-reported anger: Vulnerability for risky behaviors in two-wheeler riding young men. Indian J Psychol Med [serial online] 2019 [cited 2019 Nov 22];41:375-9. Available from: http://www.ijpm.info/text.asp?2019/41/4/375/257375



Aggressive driving or driver aggression is one of the contributing factors for road accidents, and it has emerged as one of the major concerns in the public health sector.[1] Aggressive forms of behavior on the road can be categorized into (i) aggressive behavior in which the intention is to cause physical and/or psychological harm or damage to oneself, other road users, or property and (ii) aggressive behavior in which there is no intention to harm others but the behavior appears to be risky.[2] Another term which is often used interchangeably in place of aggressive driving is road rage, and it broadly refers to driving-related aggressive behaviors on the road.[3]

There is an unprecedented growth in the number of two-wheeler riders and bicyclists worldwide and in India, as two wheelers and bicycles offer cost-effective and convenient means of transportation.[4],[5] As a result, riders and pillion riders of two-wheelers and bicyclists are one of the most vulnerable groups with respect to road traffic accidents.[4],[5]

Individual factors such as trait anger or difficulty to manage anger could be one of the important factors for aggressive driving behaviors on the road.[6],[7] Similarly, an individual's coping ability, particularly the ability to regulate one's emotion, may contribute significantly to an aggressive expression on the road. It has been found that drivers who have high anger use less adaptive coping strategies compared with those with low anger.[7] In this context, one's beliefs about having the ability to overcome or alleviate a negative mood state, also known as negative mood regulation expectancies,[8] could be an important variable from a research perspective. Literature suggests that difficulty in managing anger may increase the probability of risky or aggressive behaviors, whereas effective emotion regulation is associated with safe driving behaviors.[9] Research also indicates that age and gender are other important variables in aggressive driving. It is reported that driver violence is more prevalent in male and young drivers.[1],[10]

However, there is a paucity of research exploring driver anger and its contribution to aggressive driving in two-wheeler riding young men. This study was aimed at examining the extent to which self-report of difficulty in managing anger in general (self-perceived problematic anger) is associated with aggressive and risky riding behaviors in two-wheeler riding young men. In addition, the differences in negative mood regulation efficacy between individuals reporting problematic anger versus those not reporting problematic anger were also examined.


   Methods Top


Participants and procedure

The study used convenient sampling. The sample comprised 433 young male two-wheeler riders, undergraduate and postgraduate students, from Bengaluru city, recruited from nine colleges, including governmental and private colleges. Ethical approval was obtained from the Institute Ethics Committee. Permission for sample recruitment was obtained from the head of the college. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Demographic data sheet

A demographic data sheet was prepared for the study to document demographic information and to document variables related to two-wheeler riding (e.g. total years of two-wheeler riding and riding frequency).

Two-wheeler riding survey

A two-wheeler riding survey was developed as per the need of the study and considering the nonavailability of tools in the Indian context. Domains and items were constructed on the basis of three focus group discussions that involved college students and a literature review. The items were reviewed for content validation by three mental health professionals having more than 15 years of clinical service, and their suggestions were taken into account in finalizing the survey items. The survey items captured various aspects related to two-wheeler riding. The items pertaining to the present study objectives are briefly described below.

Participants' perception of managing anger as a problem, in general, was assessed with a discrete response-type item having three options (“yes,” “no,” and “cannot say”). Provocability to anger on the road and difficulty to control anger while riding were assessed by two items having a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from “Not at all,” scored as 1, to “Very much,” scored as 4. Response to frustrating situations on the road was assessed by 11 items related to verbal and nonverbal aggressive expressions, with a 4-point Likert scale format. The items were preceded by a general stem, “When I feel irritated or angry on the road I tend to …” (e.g., sound my horn repeatedly, give an angry look at the other driver who caused me irritation). These items together formed a scale with good internal consistency and reliability (Cronbach's alpha = 0.75). Furthermore, factors associated with riding fast than usual were assessed using a checklist with eight options, while perceived motives for riding fast than usual (in terms of seeking specific emotional states by fast riding) were assessed using five items (e.g., to get relief from anger, to get a sense of joy), 4-point Likert scale, ranging from “Never true for me,” scored as 1, to “Nearly always true for me,” scored as 4.

Negative Mood Regulation efficacy scale [8]

The Negative Mood Regulation (NMR) scale has 30 items, with a 5-point Likert rating designed to measure generalized expectancies for alleviating negative mood.[8] This scale has been tested for its psychometric properties in an Indian study, and internal consistency and reliability of the scale were found to be 0.87 (Cronbach's alpha).[11]

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences, version 15 for Windows (SPSS-15, SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA). Descriptive statistics, independent t-test, Chi-square test, and Mann–Whitney rank U test were applied to examine demographic variables and to compare the two groups of those with problematic anger and those without problematic anger.


   Results Top


Sample characteristics

The sample size of two-wheeler young men riders was 433. Participant's age ranged between 17 and 26 years with a mean of 20.21 years [standard deviation (SD) =1.63 years]. Participants were predominantly undergraduate students [344 (79.6%)], and only 89 (20.4%) were postgraduate students. The mean number of years of education was 14.67 years (SD = 1.17 years). On an average, the participants had about 5 years of two-wheeler riding experience (mean = 4.62 years, SD = 2.85 years), and the majority [355 (82%)] reported riding two-wheelers almost daily to a few days in a week.

Subjective perception of problematic anger, in general, was assessed on a single self-reported item, “Do you feel that managing anger is a problem for you in general?” with three options (i.e., yes, no, and cannot say). Of 433 participants, 83 (19.71%) reported “yes,” whereas 175 (40.42%) reported “no,” and 175 (40.42%) reported “cannot say” to this item. Two groups were formed based on the participants' responses: (i) the problematic anger-present group (i.e., those who reported “yes,” n = 83) and the problematic anger-absent group (i.e., those who reported “no,” n = 175). These groups were compared on various factors related to aggressive driving behavior as described below.

Self-report of provocability to anger and difficulty in managing anger on the road

The problematic anger-present and problematic anger-absent groups were compared on 4-point, self-reported item about easy provocability to anger on roads (“I'm easily provoked to anger while riding two-wheeler”) and difficulty in managing anger while riding (“I find it difficult to control my anger while riding”). Forty percent (n = 33) of the participants in the problematic anger-present group, whereas only 9% (n = 16) participants in the problematic anger-absent group, reported considerable to high provocability to anger, and statistical analysis revealed a significant difference between the groups [χ2df-3 = 63.94; P < 0.001 (two-tailed)].

Similarly, 39.6% (n = 33) participants in the problematic anger-present group and 11% (n = 20) in the problematic anger-absent group reported considerable to high difficulty in managing anger while riding, and there was a significant difference between the groups [χ2df-3 = 45.41; P < 0.001 (two-tailed)].

Expression of anger to frustrations on the road while riding

We compared the mean score of problematic anger-present group and problematic anger-absent group on the scale capturing aggressive responses to frustration when riding. The results showed a significant difference between the two groups [P < 0.001 (two-tailed); problematic anger-present group = 21.19 ± 5.89 and problematic anger-absent group = 18.48 ± 5.01 (mean ± SD)]. [Figure 1]a shows the significant differences between the groups on the items of this scale such as (i) give an angry look at the other driver, (ii) speed up to frustrate the other driver, and (iii) thoughts of taking revenge.
Figure 1: (a) Responses to frustrating situations on road. (b) Factors for riding fast than usual. (c) Motives for riding fast than usual. ***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05 (Two tailed); Prb.anger-present – Problematic anger-present group, Prb.anger-absent – Problematic anger-absent group

Click here to view


Perceived factors associated with riding fast than usual

There was a significant difference between the problematic anger-present and problematic anger-absent groups on topmost reasons which they perceived as factors for riding fast than usual. [Figure 1]b shows that the problematic anger-present group reported riding fast than usual due to being in a negative mood state such as anger or feeling sad. Similarly, the problematic anger-present group reported riding fast to seek relief from anger or feeling upset [Figure 1]c.

Negative mood regulation efficacy

The group reporting problematic anger, in general, had lower scores (mean rank = 90.56) compared with problematic anger-absent group (mean rank = 125.12) on NMR scale. Shapiro–Wilk test was applied to check the normal distribution and it was found to be not normally distributed. Mann–Whitney U test revealed that there was a significant difference between the two groups (P = 0.01) indicating a lower efficacy in negative mood regulation in the problematic anger-present group.


   Discussion Top


The results indicate that participants who reported experiencing problematic anger in general also reported having easy provocability to anger, difficulty in controlling anger, and a lower efficacy in negative mood regulation. These findings have implications for behaviors such as aggressive driving and road rage which may place the riders as well as others at risk. Road rage is a rising problem that is seen in the context of traffic density,[1] and such tendencies may likely place such individuals at higher risk than those who are able to regulate negative emotions and are not easily provoked. Studies have reported a significant difference between drivers who are high or low in anger.[7],[12] Individuals with low-trait anger are generally calmer when encountering provocative and annoying situations, whereas high-trait anger individuals tend to get easily provoked and lose control in the face of such situations.[13]

According to trait-state anger theory,[14] individuals with high anger trait when compared with low anger trait differ in several dimensions, for example, becoming angered by a wide spectrum of situations/events, experiencing anger more frequently and intensely, expressing anger in less adaptive ways, and experiencing more negative consequences. Furthermore, low NMR expectancies in the problematic anger-present group could suggest a higher level of anger and distress as well as difficulty in mood regulation compared with problematic anger-absent group. One study has reported that having difficulty in emotion regulation was associated with risky driving and vice versa.[9] This is particularly more important in the case of a young driver who may experience intense emotion and may have a relatively high probability of accidents.[15] Hence, it is necessary that one should have the ability to regulate negative mood.

This is further supported by the findings that participants in the problematic anger-present group reported greater use of aggressive and less constructive forms of expressing their anger in response to commonly occurring and their personally most provocative situations on the road, for example, angry look to the other driver and thinking about taking revenge. These findings are in line with several other studies that demonstrate a relationship between driver anger and risky behaviors.[7],[12],[16] Similarly, difficulty in negative emotion regulation may result in expressing negative emotion toward others and at times, more maladaptive behaviors on the road. Studies have shown that drivers with higher levels of anger use less adaptive coping methods.[7],[14],[16] Hence, difficulties in emotion regulation may increase the likelihood of aggressive or risky driving behaviors on the road.[9]

This study revealed that problematic anger-present group reported to speed up or ride fast than usual to get relief from anger and sadness. It is reported that negative emotions such as anger and sadness may lead to risky riding/driving behaviors such as stronger acceleration and speeding.[17] Several factors may cause the driver to accelerate speed in a state of anger. For example, drivers with high trait anger were found to be riding fast when they face an impediment.[18],[19] Anger may also influence risk perception.[19] Negative emotions may produce more errors related to vehicle control and slower braking reaction time, and impairment in attention and concentration.[9],[17] Furthermore, personal characteristics play an important role in experiencing emotions and influencing driving/riding behaviors. For example, younger drivers tend to experience more anger than older ones.[6],[20]


   Conclusion Top


Findings from this study have preventive implications for road safety. Individuals with high anger are prone to risky riding and accidents. These individuals may benefit from education about vulnerability to risky behaviors and negative consequences, and from addressing anger through interventions that target emotion regulation and adaptive coping skills. This may help them in dealing with provocative situations and in reducing risky riding behaviors on the road, and that, in turn, has implications for reducing incidents of clashes and accidents.

This study has a few limitations. A single item was used for dividing the sample into problematic anger-present group and problematic anger-absent group. The study excluded 40% of the sample who reported “cannot say” on this item. The instrument used has not been validated except for face validation, and the psychometric properties are not known. Objective data (e.g., accident/injury records) and pillion-riders' report to supplement self-report could enhance the robustness of the findings. The study was limited to college-going youth in an urban context. Further studies can help in examining the generalizability of the findings to youth in other settings and age groups. Its limitations notwithstanding, the study has several implications for future research and practice.

Acknowledgements

This study was part of a larger research project funded by the Council of Scientific and Industrial Research (CSIR), New Delhi, and has been carried out at the Department of Clinical Psychology, National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences (NIMHANS), Bangalore. We acknowledge the support of the principals of the colleges for giving permission for conducting the survey and the teaching staff of respective departments/classes for facilitating data collection in classroom setting.

Financial support and sponsorship

The authors gratefully acknowledge the funding support provided by the Council of Scientific and industrial Research (CSIR) for undertaking the larger study on which this article is based. No financial interests, direct or indirect, exist for the individual contributors in connection with the content of this article. The funding agency had no involvement in the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
   References Top

1.
Smart R, Stoduto G, Mann R, Adlaf E. Road rage experience and behavior: Vehicle, exposure, and driver factors. Traffic Inj Prev 2004;5:343-8.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Grey EM, Triggs TJ, Haworth NL. Driver aggression: The role of personality, social characteristics, risk and motivation, F.O.O.R.S. Department of Transport and Communications, Editor. Canberra, Australia, 1989.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Asbridge M, Smart RG, Mann RE. Can we prevent road rage? Trauma Violence Abuse 2006;7:109-21.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Gururaj G, Gautham MS. Advancing Road Safety in India-Implementation is the Key (Summary). Bangalore: National Institute of Mental Health & Neuro Sciences; 2017. p. 60.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Bhalla, K, Mohan D. Safety of young children on motorized two-wheelers around the world: A review of the global epidemiological evidence. IATSS Res 2015;38:83-91.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Lajunen T, Parker D. Are aggressive people aggressive drivers? A study of the relationship between self-reported general aggressiveness, driver anger and aggressive driving. Accid Anal Prev 2001;33:243-55.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Deffenbacher JL, Deffenbacher DM, Lynch RS, Richards TL. Anger, aggression, and risky behavior: A comparison of high and low anger drivers. Behav Res Ther 2003;41:701-18.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Catanzaro SJ, Mearns J. Measuring generalized expectancies for negative mood regulation: Initial scale development and implications. J Pers Assess 1990;54:546-63.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Trógolo MA, Melchior F, Medrano LA. The role of difficulties in emotion regulation on driving behavior. J Behav Health Soc Issues 2014;6:107-17.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Hennessy DA, Wiesenthal DL. Aggression, violence, and vengeance among male and female drivers. Transportation Quarterly 2002;56:65-75.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Mehrotra S, Tripathi R. Affect intensity and negative mood regulation (NMR) expectancies: A preliminary Indian study. Asian J Psychiatry 2012;5:137-43.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Deffenbacher JL, Huff ME, Lynch RS, Oetting ER, Salvatore NF. Characteristics and treatment of high anger drivers. J Couns Psychol 2000;47:5-17.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Deffenbacher JL. Trait anger: Theory, findings, and implications. In: Spielberger CD, Butcher JN, editors. Advances in Personality Assessment. Vol. 9. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Deffenbacher JL. Angry drivers: Characteristics and clinical interventions. Revista Mexicana de Psicología 2009;26:5-16.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Harré N. Risk evaluation, driving, and adolescents: A typology. Dev Rev 2000;20:206-26.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Deffenbacher JL, Lynch RS, Filetti LB, Dahlen ER, Oetting ER. Anger, aggression, risky behavior, and crash-related outcomes in three groups of drivers. Behav Res Ther 2003;41:333-49.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Roidl E, Frehse B, Hoger R. Emotional states of drivers and the impact on speed, acceleration and traffic violations-a simulator study. Accid Anal Prev 2014;70:282-92.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Stephens AN, Groeger JA. Situational specificity of trait influences on drivers' evaluations and driving behaviour. Transp Res Part F: Traffic Psychol Behav 2009;12:29-39.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Mesken J, Hagenzieker MP, Rothengatter JA, de Waard D. Frequency, determinants, and consequences of different drivers' emotions: An on-the-road study using self-reports, (observed) behaviour, and physiology. Transp Res Part F 2007;10:458-75.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Parker D, Lajunen T, Summala H. Anger and aggression among drivers in three European countries. Accid Anal Prev 2002;34:229-35.  Back to cited text no. 20
    


    Figures

  [Figure 1]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
  
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Access Statistics
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this article
    Abstract
     Methods
     Results
     Discussion
     Conclusion
    References
    Article Figures

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed290    
    Printed12    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded24    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal